Appendix: Developmental Theory and Doubt (by Ronald T. Habermas)
Appendix: Abraham and Sarah had their doubts. John the Baptist and Peter expressed anxieties about their faith. Even the Lord Jesus, on the cross, raised questions concerning His relationship with the Father. In addition to identifying some of these Bible character profiles, the following section intends to provide a brief survey of relevant and related theories on doubt, utilizing the twin disciplines of theology and social science. A range of perspectives will be cited to illustrate the breadth of coverage that has been given to the topic of doubt. More restrictive, theological positions (e.g., "All doubt is wrong") will be compared and contrasted with less restrictive views (e.g., "Doubt is necessary for maturity"). The well-known "reporter's questions" will frame the organization of this overview in theology and social science. A personal experience A dozen mornings before Christmas in 1987, my five-and-one-half-year-old daughter rounded the corner of the bathroom where I was shaving and issued an unprovoked testimonial: "I don't believe in Santa," Melissa began. "And I don't believe in the Easter Bunny either." Then, with resolve, she added, "And I don't believe in angels." Catching my breath at this barrage of denials, I found myself unable to get a word in edgewise, for this second-oldest child subsequently offered a single retraction: "Oh, I guess I do believe in angels." "Why do you still believe in angels?" I questioned, not really knowing how to start. "Angels are up in heaven. When you get old and die and go up to heaven, they will be there," Melissa claimed matter-of-factly. After I noted a couple of references to angels in the Bible, she reiterated, "Yeah, that's right, the Bible says a lot about angels. I do believe in angels." My preschooler seemed finished with what she had to say, for she headed for the door. But I wanted to know more about the two other subjects that she categorically denied existence. "Then why don't you believe in Santa or the Easter Bunny?" I pressed. Referring to the first denial, Melissa scientifically rationalized, "I've never heard him scratching when he comes down the inside of the chimney. And I don't hear his reindeer." Turning her thoughts to the Easter Bunny, she deduced, "He really can't write names [i.e., on Easter cards] and stuff like that. He can't make all those baskets that quick. And he doesn't have money to buy those things [Easter candy, etc.]." End of discussion. And beginning of a new growth phase for Melissa. Just what was it that brought out this confession of denials in my daughter? How long had she been struggling with these issues? Did the ideas just suddenly "make sense" to her? Were her doubts simply a natural part of growing up or were they more like the green-eyed monster from Shakespeare's Othello? "Who?" Goethe, the world renowned thinker, claimed that the primary theme throughout human civilization has been the relentless struggle between belief and unbelief. In certain historical periods (such as the Keformalion), faith prevailed as victorwhereas on other occasions, unbelief and despair have been triumphant. Harvard's G. W. Allport admits, "Our own age, we know, is a period of doubt and negation."1 In 1985, the Gallup Organization conducted a telephone interview with more than one thousand adults concerning perceptions of their faith maturation. To a great extent, this national survey confirms the suspicion of Allport: we do live in a generation of doubt. Though the word doubt was never used by the interviewing team as such, synonymous phrases were employed throughout the survey (as, for example, the phrase "questioning early beliefs"2). The Gallup survey discovered that the majority of adults (65 percent) believe a person's faith "should" change (vs. "should not") throughout life "just as one's body and mind change."^ (Only 32 percent believed faith should not change.) Church members were more convinced that faith should not change (should = 58 percent; should not = 39 percent), contrasted with nonmembers (73 percent and 24 percent, respectively). A comparison of all surveyed men and women shows almost identical results in gender, as males favor change (66 percent) over no change (32 percent), and females stand 64 percent to 32 percent in their respective selections. In response to a similar question the data shows that
Asked about actual experiences they had, 71 percent of all respondents acknowledged that their "faith changed significantly" at some time in their recent or remote past. Only 29 percent stated that they had never experienced such a change.' When they were asked about the results of such a significant faith change, "the majority experiencing a change [reported! that their faith "was" stronger (82 percent) and more meaningful (81 percent) as a consequence." "Also as a result of this change, 49 percent of the national group believed that their faith was "a little different," 45 percent found their faith to be "totally different," and 6 percent had no opinion.7 But rather than highlighting a new trend, the Gallup survey simply affirms a historic pattern. For even the early church Father Tertullian expressed his personal anxiety over the tensions caused by doubt. On the one hand, he acknowledged that, a searching faith (prompted by doubt) produced certain rewards. On the other hand, he bemoaned the futility of doubt's endeavors:
Religious educator Leon McKenzie concludes that doubt is a generic condition of num. just as natural as the functioning of the human senses. Moreover, doubt is an ever-present reality: "We come into the world with question marks in our heads. We strive for intelligibility and purpose. We seek a perspective or framework for our being-in-the-world." McKenzie then promises, "The question marks in our heads are never fully erased."9 Furthermore, several prominent studies indicate that this human condition of doubt is not limited to adulthood. Since doubt originates, in part, from the more pervasive intellectual (or cognitive) development of all persons, children and youth often experience very serious questioning of their faith as well. In fact (as it will be later shown), the period of adolescence typically brings about the most tumultuous faith struggles. With this in mind, psychology professor Michael Chandler observes that "between childhood and maturity there automatically occurs a deeply problematic period of epistemological confusion, marked by the collapse of absolute conviction and defined by an outcropping of nascent skeptical doubt" (emphasis added).10 Is there anyone who is immune to this perceived nocuous condition? If there is, it tends not to be the Christian. For in the believer's sincerest attempts to search for truth and to confront the ultimate issues of life, there often appears to be some universal, ironic twistwhere the seeker turns skeptic. As an outgrowth of his doctoral work in social relations at Harvard, Philip M. Helfaer observes that "doubt and the seeds of doubtdespair, skepticism, and angry question of God's justicearc the central themes in the Judeo-Christ inn tradition." Prominent examples, he says, include Job's "sense of abandonment," "the Preacher's dry skepticism in Ecclesiastes," and Jesus' own lost cry of Matthew 27:46 (King James Version): "My Clod, my God, why has thou forsaken me?"11 Echoing this observation, Allport has noted that "the mature religious sentiment is ordinarily fashioned in the workshop of doubt."12 Even members of the esteemed profession of theology must succumb to this human tendency, as Protestant leader Karl Barth testifies:
"what?" Doubt has been broadly defined as "the calling into question of either beliefs or practices of one's religious tradition, or of organized religion in general."" Whether or not this definition is employed, one thing is certain: the subject of doubt is a volatile one. As such, it becomes imperative that certain misnomers or incorrect designations for doubt be identified and rejected as misleading. WHAT DOUBT IS NOT First, doubt is not necessarily sin. Christian educator Elmer Towns states that, contrary to some interpretations, "Eve did not sin when she doubted God, but when she disobeyed God. Today, doubts simply cannot be eradicated, even by the miracle of the new birth."l5 To look at it another way, within the context of Scripture there is a marked difference between the "doubt" of the Pharisees (see Matt. 21:23-27) and the "doubt" of John the Baptist (see Matt. 11:1-6). In the first case, a "don't confuse me with the facts" mentality is expressed. In the latter case, however, John's teachable attitudein the midst of his doubtcould be paraphrased "Please help me to resolve this complex faith problem." Second, doubt is not necessarily disbelief or denial. Barth comments, "Doubt only means swaying and staggering between Yes and No. It is only an uncertainty."16 Allport elaborates this fact through a composite of comparison and contrast:
Towns further illustrates this contrast: "Doubt is not. unbelief. Unbelief is rebellion against evidence that we cannot or will not accept. Doubt is stumbling over a stone that we do not understand. Unbelief is kicking at a stone that we understand all too well. 18 Third, doubt is not necessarily detrimental. Columbia University's Philip Phenix offers a helpful distinction between differing types of doubt. He labels the first category "constructive doubt and faith," further describing the phrase as "faithful doubt"a curious (if not contradictory) statement for some believers. Phenix proposes this description based upon "Tillich's reformulation of Luther's doctrine of justification by faith in a state of sin to read justification by faith in a state of doubt."19 To put it in other terms, Phenix believes that questioning Christians could claim "the secure foundation of the human condition as a spiritual being" by participating in "the faith-evidencing activity of concerned and responsible doubting."20 Using a comparative analogy, Phenix titles his second category "destructive doubt." This educator comments that his experiences would classify persons in the latter group as closed-minded, since they are
TWO SCHOOLS OF THOUGHT It goes without saying that liberal believers would regard doubt as a more natural and acceptable phenomenon, whereas extremely conservative Christians seem to brand all doubt as wrong. More than mere suspicion, however, this statement has been documented by certain research findings. For instance, Hugh Alien studied more than three hundred college undergraduates, trying to determine (among other concerns) what correlation might exist between individual doubts and participation in religious activities. He discovered that collegiates of more conservative denominations (in his study, this included Methodists and Baptists) generally experienced more disruptive consequences of doubting than their counterparts in liberal denominations who questioned their faith. One of the more sobering conclusions Alien reached included evidence that even following a battle with doubt, faith struggles failed to dissipate.
Sometimes it boils down to the fact that certain fellowships simply ostracize those who have struggled with doubts, like those who have been divorced or those who have suffered some paralyzing illness or handicap. Believers often do not know how to respond in a facilitating manner. A young man recently wrote to me about doubt . and concluded: "Many Christians avoid this topic like the , plague, fearing it will somehow infect and conquer them." In a similar vein, one young woman corresponded that she perceived the church was weighed down and "burdened with doubts." Yet, ironically, she had learned that it was foolhardy to generally expect any assistance from the Body of Christ. Her frank assessment is bone-chilling: "It is just about the worst place in the world for someone hurting. I got kinder responses from bar keepers." One school of thought stresses: any doubt is compatible with faith. Some proponents would say that, like water combined with oil, any rationalization (which often leads to doubt) mixed with belief is totally antithetical to faith--is totally unacceptable. For them, it would seem that "religious commitment must transcend categories of ~ rational justification if it is not to be undermined by the criticism which is the obverse of such justification."23 This rationale is further explained by Helfaer, who states that right wing theology "rejects doubt partly for ! the very reason that doubt represents open interchange between the individual and his world and a change in conception of the world in the direction of expanding its contents and meanings in the light of experience."24 On the other side of the coin, there stands a second school of thought that emphasizes: doubts are not only compatible with faith but are actually imperative for faith to grow. One existentialist subgroup of this belief focuses on (be individual and his or her particular religious commitment. As a primary spokesman for this position, theologian Paul Tillich summarizes: "Existential doubt and faith are poles of the same reality, the stale of ultimate ' concern. . . . Serious doubt is confirmation of faith."25 In comparison with the first school of thought (which favors a more dualistic view of reality concerning faith and reason), religious existentialism partially bases its perspective on man's finiteness. Tillich explains: "Finitude includes doubt. . . . It is an expression of the acceptance of his [man's] finitude that he accepts the fact that doubt belongs to his essential being. . . . Doubt is not the opposite of faith; it is an element of faith."26 Another subgroup valuing the compatibility of faith and human inquiry would also emphasize a more rationalistic stance for beliefs. Yet, they would not be branded as existentialists, by any means. In fact, many proponents would hold to a conservative theology. Virtually everyone in this ideological camp adheres to the following summative declaration: "Questioning my beliefs and even doubting their truth need not necessarily weaken my commitment."27 The mature believer, from this vantage point, uses his or her renewed mind but does not foolishly abandon the tenets of faith for every minute question. To paraphrase Cardinal Newman's saying, even ten thousand faith struggles do not justify giving up on a belief, if you have nothing better to replace it.28 For example, Towns speaks about a missionary who may doubt God's work in his life because of some misfortune or unanswered prayer. Rather than a response of faith denial, Towns suggests that the root cause may stem from this worker's ignorance of the true meaning of the life of faith. As an evangelical, Towns admits that rationalism cannot provide the final answer for believers. Yet Towns observes that faith struggles can often be helped by clear and logical thinking. In chorus, he shares the same tune as the religious existentialist (though from a markedly different theological position) when he summarizes, "Doubt comes when we do not know all of the answers."29 Tennyson put it this way: "There lives more faith in honest doubt,/Believe me, than in half the creeds." In summary, (hen, rationalization and doubt are typically viewed as part and parcel of faith development--or they are not. DIFFERENTIATING TERMS One way to better comprehend (he nature of doubt is (o further study the essence of faith through its various descriptions. In his helpful book Religion and Doubt: Toward a Faith of Your Own Richard R. Creel provides a provocative comparison between four terms that arc often used synonymously to describe religious commitment. The distinctions Creel employs are helpful in clarifying the subject of doubt. The first synonym Creel discusses is the word knowledge. He describes this term as a claim held to be "absolutely and demonstrably true."30 Consequently, this religious conviction can be "proved" to anyone who is intelligent and unbiased enough to consider the evidence. For this first category, Creel offers the example of the Roman Catholic church's position regarding the certainty of God's existence (derived from the five proofs of Aquinas in the thirteenth century). The second synonym Creel discusses is the term belief. Likewise based upon supportive evidence (though difficult to "prove beyond a shadow of a doubt"), this category of religious commitment can be described as "the most plausible explanation of the nature of reality." Creel further explains, "In other words, given the evidence presently at hand, you believe that your religion is more likely the true explanation of reality than any other explanation with which you are familiar."" The pivotal issue here is that religious commitment is based upon the highest probability of alternative views. Faith represents the third category of religious terminology, defined as emotional confidence. From this perspective there are
Creel claims that the term faith is not the consequence of an exerted will or intentional reasoning--it just "happens." The fourth and final synonym used to describe religious commitment is hope. Distinguishing this stance from the earlier trio of options, Creel says: "You would not be claiming it to be true or logical on empirical grounds; nor would you be claiming to feel as though it is true."33 On a more positive note, he concludes, "You would be claiming only that you hope it is true and that you believe that there is a possibility that it is true."34 The summary chart on the following page (adapted from Creel) contrasts these four alternative descriptions of religious commitment. In addition, the diagram cites areas that would tend to cause doubt in each case. As Creel has pointed out, the dotted line on the chart indicates the degree to which human reason is utilized. That is, the first two categories assume that reason can be employed to identify ultimate reality. The latter two categories have little use for reason, relying more on emotion. It should he noted at this juncture that we authors do not strictly adhere to Creel's four-part categorization. A major concern of ours, for instance, is that faith and doubt should not be viewed dualistically by separating rational from emotional categories. Also, some of his interpretations would be questionable in our minds. However, Creel's contribution indicates that he has seriously grappled with the what of doubt. He has attempted to isolate the many words we use as synonyms and to become more discerning in conceptualization and communication.
Fluctuation between human reason and emotion as the primary source or basis for religious commitment was verified in the 1985 Gallup study. When respondents were asked, "Would you describe the change [in your faith] mostly as coming about as a result, of a lot of thought and discussion about faith or coming about as a result of a strong emotional experience?" they were about equally divided in their responses. Forty-six percent of the national group chose the first phrase, and 49 percent identified with the second. There was no appreciable difference between all males (48 percent to 40 percent, respectively, in these two categories) and all females (45 percent to 50 percent, respectively). However, education did appear to be a factor. Among all college graduates, 61 percent chose the rational component, compared with '34 percent for the emotional component, whereas 41 percent of high school graduates selected the former category and 53 percent opted for the latter category.35 "when? where? why?" When does doubt occur? In the 1985 Gallup survey, nearly six out often persons (39 percent) reported that a change in their faith came during a stable time of life, whereas 40 percent , said that faith change arose during turbulent, chaotic periods.'" Also, based upon the 71 percent who claimed to have had a significant faith change, 18 percent recalled that their experience came when they were eighteen 1o twenty-four y nil's of age (I he age I > nickel wild I he highest recorded percentage). In descending order, other age periods included the following: 9 percent who cited a faith change when they were twenty-five to twenty-nine years of age, 7 percent who pointed to such a change identified the years when they were sixteen to seventeen years of age, and 6 percent noted that the faith change occurred when they were thirty to thirty-four years of age. Besides age, when are doubts most likely to occur? James E. Marcia suggests a provocative theory of older teens and younger adults, which reflects helpful insights for this query.37 Concisely stated, Marcia poses two primary factors determining personal identity formation. Commitment and crisis. Commitment focuses upon the degree of allegiance to beliefs and life values. Occupational preferences and choices are also included. Crisis represents any meaningful decision-making moment that facilitates commitment. Using this duo as perpendicular axes creates a Z x Z matrix like the one on the following page. For both the vertical and horizontal axes, the subcategory Yes indicates actual experience. The subcategory No points out inexperience. The combination of factors yields four options. Marcia's theory provides the following categories and technical terms.
Identity diffusion means that an individual has not developed significant values about life or held prominent vocational preferences (i.e., no commitment). Also, no crisis has been encountered. Moratorium indicates that the individual has confronted a decision-making moment. Indeed, research shows that this person often tends to be churning in the middle of crisis. Consequently, the debilitating struggle (which may involve faith-defying experiences) "freezes" the young person. No major commitment can be secured. Foreclosure. This is the flip side of Moratorium. It defines a state where beliefs and vocational decisions have been made, but they have come without any thought-provoking crises. For example, personal convictions here are based upon moral codes and opinions of outsiders (e.g., parents, church leaders, and other authority figures). Extrinsically-oriented values. Finally, Identity and Achievement stands as Marcia's technical term for one who has "owned" personal values. Through the refining fire of crisis, genuine commitment is derivedintrinsic beliefs. Adapting Marcia's research and terminology to the study of doubts, it would appear that substantial faith questioning primarily occurs in two of the four quadrants. "Moratorium" designates the spiritual condition of one wrestling with the beliefs--much like the unstable person portrayed by the apostle James: "a double-minded man." Their chaos is likened to a "wave of the sea, blown and tossed by the wind" (James 1:6-8, KJV). No wonder value commitment is nowhere to be found. "Identity Achievement" typifies the second spiritual state in which doubts normally occur. In (.his case, however, growth and stability reign. Struggling through doubts has paradoxically strengthened this individual. In fact, analogy can be drawn from James's opening comments, where a rather curious spiritual equation is introduced: "Consider it pure joy, my brothers, whenever you face trials of many kinds" (1:2), for trials test faith. Perseverance comes, and eventually maturity results (vv. 3- 4). An earlier research project by Marcia verifies the basic faith-doubt implications cited above.38 At one point in his survey, the question was raised, "Have you ever had any doubts about your religious beliefs?" Representative responses indicated cluster themes around each of Marcia's four categories, as the following samples suggest: [Identity achievement] Yea, I even started wondering whether or not there was a god. I've pretty much resolved that now, though. The way it seems to me is ... All port highlights al least a half-do/en instances where serious questioning of faith begins. First, reactive and negativistic doubt often surfaces when tragedies are encountered. The foxhole prayer that goes unanswered frequently results in militant atheism, souring any form of religious activity. Second, extreme egocentrism (a relentless focus on self) often brings doubt with it. Childish expectations of faith in adulthood that are never fully resolved may cause an individual to "discard his conceptions and terminate once and for all his religious quest," Allport notes. Concerning the eventual outcome of this type of doubt, Allport adds, "A faith centered in self-advantage is bound to break up."39 Hypocrisy and failure within institutional religion induces a third form of doubt. In particular, doubt of this sort affects young people "who today seem supersensitive to the darker spots of religious history." For, even after bearing the standard excuses for the church's failure, Allport reports, doubters are still not "persuaded by the counter-argument that crimes of persecution and bigotry are to be charged up to secularism and corruption rather than to the religious hypothesis in its purity."40 The fourth type of doubt parallels the seed (based upon the parable of the Sower) that falls upon the rocky soil and is ultimately choked of its faith-life. In the gospel story, initial joy may he compared with "religious strivings . . . the pursuit of meanings beyond the range of our intellectual capacity . . . the longing that, values be conserved."41 But as the seed's eventual demise results from shallow soil, so doubt arises in this fourth case because the shallowness of its immature faith persists in retaining juvenile perceptions of being dependent upon others. In contrast to this fourth doubt form, Allport observes that "when well-formed, the mature religious sentiment develops a driving power in its own right, motivating action, transforming character, and ordering sub-systems of belief and conduct."42 Fifth, doubts occur because of scientific or rational scrutiny. Allport contests that, in theory, the concept of an "open-minded scientist" (i.e., one who thinks exclusively in scientific and rationalistic structures) is merely an illusion. "In playing the game of science, a definite set of axioms must be adopted. They include, first and foremost, the principle of determinism." "Identical happenings," he continues, "with identical histories, will have identical futures. There can be no intervention of Providence, . . . The axiom of determination must always be held; otherwise the game of science, as now conceived, cannot be played."43 In other words, a world view that presupposes the superiority of scientific discovery and human reason (to the exclusion of supernatural reality) is potentially doomed to suffer doubtsince the glue that holds together life's puzzle has been discarded. The sixth and last form of faith questioning that All-port suggests merely reflects a watered down version of the previous categorya generic brand of doubt, so to speak, for the nonprofessional "scientist." Typically this mind-set is claimed to be a natural, or realist, view of life. Allport describes its consequence as "the commonest mode of doubting."44 A young woman was sharing a few of her personal doubts with me, when I asked, "What advice would you give to a believer who was doubting?" Characterizing Allport's sixth category, she confidently stated, "Helax, God is in control." But. then, without pausing, she qualified her advice: "And if He's not, we're in the wrong business!" She demonstrated faith mingled with realistic questions. Specifically, Allport notes that the strife in this final category comes when particular tenets of religious teaching are pilled against so-called acceptable standards of evidence. Traditional stumbling blocks in our twentieth century age of enlightenment, for instance, include the rejection of both a literal fiery hell and a heaven with golden streets, he states. Allport recommends that in such cases appropriate literary interpretation and contextualization are often overlooked by doubters, as he illustrates: "The Bible affirms, 'There is no God' but adds, 'says the fool in his heart.' Even the Fundamentalist must take the context into account."45 Why Does Doubt Occur? The following section addresses the third divisional question (which may have causal significance) by using a trio of subtopics: (1) genetic factors affecting doubt, (2) genetic and environmental factors affecting doubt, and (3) specific reasons for doubt. Moving from general to more particular issues, (his section will catch a birds'-eye glimpse of human development theory as well as theories specifically confronting origins of religious doubt. Genetic factors affecting doubt. When a comprehensive meaning of the word doubt is stressed, many relevant theorists in the field of human development would claim that doubt naturally arises from within people. Likewise they would tend to argue that such predilection to doubt represents a singular--and inherited--component of holistic growth. That is, just as it is expected that certain physiological and psychological changes will occur within every individual throughout all of life, the process of lifelong doubt, is recognized as one more natural phenomenon of human maturation. Also, just as individual distinctions are expected within other basic human growth patterns, wide-ranging experiences of doubt, are anticipated to differ from person to person. Four areas of general human development must be considered as relevant to the subject of doubt. 1. Cognitive (or intellectual) development represents a foundational topic pertaining to faith and doubt formation. Jean Piaget, the famous Swiss-born psychologist, is said to have fashioned the modern-day approach to this discipline. Simply put, Piaget perceived that cognitive growth proceeds through a series of four stages; each new state is qualitatively distinct from the prior stage, as indicated below: Stage 1: Sensory-motor intelligence (from 0 to 2 years of age) Within all four of these stages, several complex processes of cognitive growth are operating. Five basic concepts, in particular, need to be understood to further comprehend Piagel's theory. Schema refers to the intellectual structures that people use to categorize life experiences according to common characteristics. For example, if a young child regularly calls all cats "dogs," perhaps his schema would reveal that both cab and dogs are perceived by him lo be friendly, furry, four-legged creatures. Adults, of course, would have more discerning schemata, based upon their latter stage development and personal experiences. Assimilation defines the intellectual procedure whereby a person incorporates new perceptions into current, existing schemata. Using the illustration above, consider what might happen when the same child encounters for the first time a caged lion at a zoo. If the child replies "dog" when he is asked the creature's name, that would indicate that his earlier schema has not been altered--simply one more animal has has been added to it. Accommodation represents the cognitive process that recognizes that former, existing schemata no longer adequately classify and categorize life experiences. Two options are open here. Using the above case to illustrate, either the child will need (1) to create a new schema (for example, recognizing that the lion is anything I it friendly, he may create a new cognitive category of 'features that includes animals that are both unfriendly and large) or (2) to modify existing schemata in order to account for new experiences (for example, realizing that i le lion is unfriendly, the young child might reduce the characteristic of "dogs" to just furry and four-legged creatures). Both options are examples of accommodation. Disequilibrium, the fourth of five basic concepts within cognitive studies, has particular significance for Piaget's theory and for the subject of doubt. When life experiences are not easily assimilated, they result in the tension of disequilibrium--an imbalance or instability of cognition. Barry J. Wadsworth, one of the interpreters of Piaget, concludes that "disequilibrium (a problem), always leads to active efforts to assimilate and accommodate. Disequilibrium is Piaget's primary explanatory concept of motivation."47 It may already be apparent to the reader how disequilibrium thus relates to the matter of doubt. As in the area of cognitive growth, disequilibrium may produce doubt, since it is necessary to regularly reassess current perceptions and beliefs about life. An example would be the first time a young Christian teen encounters the view that God really does not exist. That experience--what may or may not be a traumatic one--will motivate him to confront his cognitive (and faith) imbalance. Equilibrium identifies a balance between assimilation and accommodation processes. Such a balance is necessary because neither extreme is healthy, too much assimilation (with little or no accommodation) would yield a fewbut very largeschemata, since similarity is stressed, whereas too much accommodation (with little or no assimilation) would bring about a cumbersome number of very minute schemata, since dissimilarity is emphasized. The weakness of the former would be a person's inability to discern or differentiate his particular experiences, and the weakness of the latter would be the inability to group his experiences by common, larger categories of reality. 2. The second area of maturity--affective (or emotional) development--should not be so disconnected with cognitive growth that a dualistic view of people is generated; yet emotional development does represent a discrete domain of the human maturational process that must be considered separately regarding faith and doubt formation. Emotion, just like intellect, has the potential to contribute to total human growth. Likewise both have the capacity to elicit nonproductive (or counterproductive) consequences for growth. For instance, one study shows that two major types of cognitive-emotional misdirection have been identified in children. Such experiences in childhood often yield fearful and anxious resultsdoubt being just one.48 The first type has been called "errors of inference," further defined as "predictions or conclusions that falsely represent reality."49 Several illogical processes (including overgeneralization) prompt this condition. One example would be for a child who has recently lost his pet bird in death to conclude, "God must not like me very much." The second type of cognitive-emotional misdirection incorporates two parts: "ego anxiety," or worry pertaining to one's self (e.g., "I must do well in the Sunday school contest, so my teacher will think I really love Jesus"); and "discomfort anxiety," or worry pertaining to the notion that only personal discomfort is threatening (e.g., "It would ho just awful if I lost the Sunday school contest").50 But children are certainly not the only people who are controlled by such cognitive-emotional misdirection. Helfaer refers to one of his clients who was suffering from doubt through an exhibition of this identical behavior: He gives reality a very specific interpretation and uncritically suspends an openness to the possibility of other interpretations. On occasion he explicitly insists on his own interpretation regardless of an awareness of evidence that might contradict it. . . .That is, the feeling of tightness about a belief is accepted, without further criticism, as an indication of the truth of that belief. . . . The feeling of truth and reality, are higher-criteria for belief than the critical evaluation of reason. In this way faith is projected from intellectual doubt and reality-testing in general.51 In some ways, these demonstrations of emotional one-sidedness reflect the opposite human condition of the earlier-described condition of extreme scientific rationalism. 3. Moral development (from certain angles, an extension of the two previous areas of human growth, especially cognitive theory) has been brought to the public's attention largely due to the research of Harvard's Lawrence Kohlberg. His theory, like Piaget's, stresses sequential, hierarchical stages of development. In fact, since his research focuses more upon the rationale that people offer for their actions (vs. their actual moral behavior itself), the link with cognitive theory is extremely significant. An overview of Kohlberg's theory below reveals his three primary levels, each of which contains two stages. "Preconventional" level--At this first, juncture of moral maturity, the individual responds to cultural rules which are enforced by those who exercise physical power. Specifically, the focus of attention is "self," since the person is motivated by what will reduce personal punishment and gain personal reward. Through a brief analysis, it may become apparent that several cross-references In the subject of doubt are potentially expressed in Kohlberg's theory. For instance, reconsider the three primary foci of each distinct level above (i.e., a focus on self, others, and principles, respectively). At the first level, the existence of Clod might be questioned by individuals when their mechanical and egocentric view of faith is not consistently satisfied (e.g., when the false dictum "Good people should never suffer" is violated). At the second level, God's love may be doubted when an individual is treated disloyally by a trusted family member or good friend. Al the third level, disillusionment with Clod's divine order may occur either when faith is lost in the consensus process or when an individual experiences the turmoil of conflicting principles within the conscience. 4. Faith or spiritual development covers a broadly defined area of human growth, not simply the traditional topics of the Judeo-Christian heritage. James W. Fowler of Emory University stands as one of the prominent leaders in this field. Like Kohlberg, Fowler's position reveals definite ties with Piaget's cognitive theory, yet warrants a distinctive discipline all its own."53 Perhaps one of the most intriguing illustrations of Fowler's allegiance with cognitive-philosophical systems comes from his reference to the potential faith struggle that children have when they enter Piaget's first growth stage. (In review, the last cognitive stage is formal operations, often commencing in the preteen years; it provides the capacity to hypothesizeto mentally play with the abstract world of thought.) Referring to the older child's newly acclaimed ability to reorganize his perception of faith, Fowler comments, "This construction frequently gives way during a phase we have come to call eleven-year-old atheism" (emphasis added).54 Such questioning of their earlier-perceived notions of God must, again, be partially understood within the context of their cognitive, emotional, and social maturation, as Fowler further explains: This phase comes when thoughtful children whose religious and social environments have given them sufficient emotional space to question and reckon for themselves begin to come to terms with the fact that ours is not a "quick-pay-off universe." The good do not always get rewarded; the wicked are not always punished.55 By way of critiquing Fowler's explanation, someone might be quick to conclude, "If the environment that allowed the child to question his dissonant perceptions had been absent, the reassessment of the child's traditional faith would not have occurred." Although this may initially look like an accurate observation, it is actually shortsighted; for it must be fully stressed that research indicates such an environmental void would have merely meant postponement of--not escape from--inevitable faith struggles later in life. In the case of such postponement, faith development tends to be dwarfed by other areas of human growth--on imbalance that may produce devastating consequences, as Fowler observes: In fact, we see a fair number of persons--usually men --who may exhibit considerable cognitive sophistication in their occupational worlds (as physicians or engineers, for example) bill who in their emotional and faith lives are rather rigidly embedded in the structures of Mythic-Literal faith and imperial selfhood. To their marriages and family life they bring a rigidity--often coupled with authoritarian patterns--that inflicts psychic and sometimes physical violence on their partners and children. It often leads them to a kind of baffled bereftness in their forties and fifties, when in the shambles of their shattered families, for the first time they may begin the painful task of learning about the interior lives of selves--starting with their own.56 Helfaer, in his own studies on doubt, verifies this noteworthy find of Fowler's. Selecting one man from his research who particularly failed to mature in his child-hood faith, Helfaer outlines grievous patterns of doubt that were subsequently suffered in adulthood. The old faith, the earlier religious sublimations, could not possibly do the psychological work needed to contain these inner and miter threats. Since it could not provide the protection of faith, it was doubted. It is possible that the inner tension that precipitated his conversion when he was eight was the same form of anxiety that later became expressed, first in his early teens.57 Later in his text, Helfaer offers further insight into this specific case study: "The inability to re-evaluate the world view of childhood and the childhood premises upon which conflicts were resolved and the sense of self established may he considered a form of doubt."58 These findings within the faith development research once again address the complexity and interrelated features of human growth. In particular, doubts--too long relegated to isolated tidbits of religious life--have now been documented to permeate all avenues of human existence: how we think, how we feel about ourselves, how we relate to others, and how we resolve the conflict of perceptual dissonance represent but a few correlations with faith struggles. The issue, then, becomes not so much whether to confront doubt, but when to address it, as Fowler summarizes, "At thirteen, when it comes much more naturally and painlessly, or at fifty-throe, when it comes out of the agony of broken relationships."59 Texas Tech's Mark 0. Webb offers a helpful perspective at this point, since he readily acknowledges parallels between faith development and other domains of human growth. But he also cautions against overlooking dissimilarities. For example, Webb likens doubt to "an internal warning" system, observing that one common motivation of mankind is somehow to maintain a state of equilibrium. (Thus, the correlated theories of Piaget, Festinger, and others provide invaluable resources.) However, Webb refuses to neglect doubt's peculiar condition. He contrasts what he calls religious experience and "normal doubt-resolution," which, he says, includes questions arising from secular and scientific investigations. Webb submits that the latter category of questioning permits objective evidence to be gathered and a choice between two alternatives to be made, whereas the former category, he claims, does not permit such convenience of scientific evaluation, given the nature of faith. This dualistic distinction seems somewhat contrived. Nothing is mentioned, for instance, of the subjective presumptions of scientific methods and tasks earlier noted by Allport. Nevertheless, Webb's contribution here arises from his insight that religious doubt does pose a particular problem, since oftentimes the core issue of religious doubt "is not subject to normal empirical modes of inquiry."60 Ranging far beyond such restrictive forms of scientific investigation, metaphysical questions (concerning the reality of a personal God and His handiwork) typically lie at the base of faith questioning. Webb concludes: "No method of resolving the doubt presents itself, since these things cannot be investigated by looking at the world. Consequently, the doubt remains, growing in intensity over time." Recalling that an equilibrated human condition is naturally sought, Webb observes that "a radical readjustment of the whole belief system takes place under the pressure of persistent doubt. That is why we have intense religious experiences, but only rarely do we have intense scientific experiences."61 Genetic and environmental factors affecting doubt. Building upon this single influence of heredity, many developmental theorists directly or indirectly agree with the assertion that "doubt" naturally arises from the combination of inherited traits, self-percept ion and social interaction. Erik II. Erikson perhaps best represents a prominent figure who values these complementary factors of personal and environmental features. Consequently, a brief summary of Erikson's theory will be presented--especially emphasizing his theory's first half, which discusses children and youth. Ramifications will be drawn for the subject of doubt. First, it is important to note that Erikson modifies and expands Sigmund Freud's five stages of psychosexual theory, in offering his eight stages of psychosocial theory. In further contrast to Freud (who primarily concentrated on birth through adolescence) Erikson postulates a life-span view for the total age range of human growth. Erikson believes that individuals develop healthily when particular life challenges of each stage are successfully negotiated. In order to value the dynamics of life, each of his eight stages is intentionally framed by a set of polarities (or tensions) highlighting particular life challenges. Also, based upon his research, Erikson includes a synthesizing virtue for every one of his stages. Erikson's theory of eight stages is presented below through a sequencing of four major periods of life.62 1. Early childhood. Erikson's first stage pitted the tension of "Trust versus Mistrust" (from birth to approximately eighteen months). Here, the infant is challenged by the need to trust others when private needs (e.g., hunger) are met. Conversely, distrust results when uncertainty and anxiety arise, as private needs go unattended. Erikson further describes the negative side of this stage as "that 'double take' which we call doubt--doubt in himself and doubt in the firmness and perspicacity of his trainers."63 The virtue that is anticipated in the resolution of this tension is a new condition called "hope." Stage 2 (from eighteen months to three years of age) highlights the challenge of "Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt." Positive resolution of this tension should yield self-worth and the virtue of "will," as the child's environment supports self-insistence. Erikson rephrases this polarity as a struggle "between self-expression and compulsive self-restraint or meek compliance. . . . From an unavoidable sense of loss of self-control and of parental over control comes a lasting propensity for doubt and shame."64 Implications for religious doubt can be projected from Erikson's additional commentary on self assessment: "This basic sense of doubt... is the model for the habitual 'double take' or other later and more verbal forms of compulsive doubting."65 "Initiative versus Guilt" designates the third stage of psychosocial theory, incorporating ages three to six. The focus is on creating and instigating individual activities, leading to virtues of "purpose" and accomplishment. Fear of punishment or guilt stands at the opposite pole and is experienced as the young child is overcome by potential failure (even perceived failure) from such self-initiated activities. 2. School-age childhood. As the young child enters school, the start of Erikson's fourth stage, two major changes are experienced within a matter of years. First, a radical shift in social activities occurs. Prior to school age, peer interactionparticularly in larger groupswas minimal (excluding preschool or daycare experiences). During school age, approximately one-half of the child's waking hours are spent with other small children. This shift in social interaction will be challenged by the ideas, needs, and demands of his peers. Wadsworth summarizes: "Social behavior, by its very nature, is an important source of disequilibrium, doming to look al something from another's viewpoint, questioning one's reasoning, and seeking validation from others are all essentially acts of accommodation."66 The age-relevant need here, among others, is to seek recognition and approval of others (for the first, time on a broader scale). In fact, in simple terms, the research that discovered this need led Erikson to the conclusion that children proceed with self-evaluation (who they are), based upon self-accomplishment (what (hey do). He officially labels this fourth stage "Industry versus Inferiority," for six-year olds up through the start of puberty. Industry translates into comprehending the value of work as well as the attainment of technological skills. Positive self-images, therefore, are constructed when schoolchildren feel good about the social support they receive for individual achievement. Conversely, they sense inferior attitudes when they experience self-defeating frustration and failure in their work. "Competence" is the intended virtue of resolution in this fourth stage crisis. Besides the radical shift in the degree of social interaction, the second major change in school-age children comes as they develop the ability to think logically and rationally (Piaget's third of four stages). Contrasting their earlier preschool cognitive competencies, Chandler notes, So long as meanings were imagined to he features of objects rather than subjects, it was easy enough to suppose that the facts would remain the same regardless of who was in the business of collecting them. As meanings come to be understood as mental products that are actively manufactured rather than harvested as natural resources, however, the idea of absolute truth is emptied of much of its earlier significance, and the companion notion of objectivity deteriorates [emphasis added].67 In other words, certain specific doubts will arise in the school-age child simply because of the cognitive movement from semi-logical to logical thinking. (That is precisely what had happened to my daughter Melissa, in the introductory story about her personal doubts.) Chandler has stated that this doubting process may have snowballing consequences. Using quite a vivid illustration, he concludes: "The effect of pulling on this small thread of insight is to eventually unravel the whole epistemic [i.e., nature of knowing] fabric of middle childhood."68 3. Adolescence. The potential for destructive, self-critical thinking continues when puberty is reached. Erikson calls this "a transitory total self-doubt."69 One hopeful sign and recent finding for this period, however, may indicate that, such self-doubt decreases as "adolescents become increasingly certain of the traits and characteristics they attribute to themselves. [It involves] a gradual consolidation of self-evaluations."70 Regardless, doubt does advance to a new challenge, for young teenagers typically move into Piaget's fourth and final stage of formal operations (i.e., the ability to think abstractly and hypothesize). At (he same (hue, they have entered Erikson's fifth stage, "Identify versus Hole Confusion"--the stage holding "fidelity" as its anticipated virtue in resolution. That is, as the early teen begins to understand who he or she is, the teen continues to experiment with a variety of potential roles in order to fine tune identity. If psychosocial pressures and demands override a positive comprehension of self-in-the-world, confusion and despair reign in the teen years. Chandler depicts the multi-faceted struggles of adolescence in this manner: The price of all this new-found uncertainly is generic doubt, not the kind of mundane, case-specific doubt of middle childhood, but a wholesale, transcendental kind of doubt that threatens to annihilate the whole of one's system of beliefs. . . . What are novel to I he adolescent period are those more unassuageable, universal doubts that have their roots in the remote conjectural possibility of hypothetical error. . . . Discovering some route around this impasse, some means of recovering an acceptable epistemological footing in an essentially uncertain world, it would be argued, is a primary developmental task of the adolescent period.71 This "generic doubt" is elsewhere described as the "recognition of the universality of subjectivity,"72 and for conservative Christians a totally relative world view is neither acceptable nor reassuring, Employing a horribly graphic metaphor for this age period, Chandler defines such cognitive (and faith) turmoil as a time when "acquired knowledge is the epistemological equivalent of a bullet in the brain."73 To make matters worse, Chandler adds that his studies reveal that normative, adolescent reaction to these stressful times yields one of two outcomes: "either a blind dogmatic faith or a know-nothing skepticism."74 Certainly, more than those two options exist, but Chandler's summative remarks, nevertheless, provide a startling commentary of youth who are overwhelmed by doubt. 4. Adulthood. Particularly recounting the second stage of "Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt," Erikson observes that adult faith questioning will be influenced by the degree of successful stage negotiation during early childhood maturation. Developmentally stated, Erikson asserts that the child's potential for "this basic sense of doubt. . . finds its adult expression in paranoiac fears."75 For the believer, Helfaer provides a helpful correlation between Erikson's theory and spiritual development, saying that "basic mistrust, shame and doubt, arid guilt can all be given symbolic expression in the terms of religious belief."76 An additional example of this connectedness includes the fact that one of the underlying themes of Erikson's research on Martin Luther was that (lie Reformer's attempt to assess mid affirm his own faith was based upon his experienced trust in early infancy." In brief, the three adult psychosocial stages of Erikson are: Stage 6, "Intimacy versus Isolation," when younger adults seek serious commitments with others or withdraw socially, becoming self-absorbed; Stage 7, "Generativity versus Stagnation," where middle adults attempt to care for the next generation or continue in further forms of isolation, through self-indulgence; and Stage 8, "Ego Integrity versus Despair," as older adults review and accept their life accomplishments or become depressed and anxious over disapproving self-evaluations of life (virtues include "love," "care," and "wisdom," respectively). To follow through on the comparison Helfaer made, during any one of these three adult stages the potential for storing up or breaking down faith is possible. Specific reasons for doubt. The third of three subtopics that confront the issue, Why does doubt occur? focuses on more particular matters of religious doubt (over against the generic doubt of the human condition). A handful of studies have been conducted among those who suffer from religious doubt. From preliminary glances at such research, there exists no rhyme or reason for the causal source of spiritually disenchanted persons. For example, in Helfaer's studies alone, just a few of the complementary influences associated with doubt range from low self-esteem78 to a lack of repentance,79 and from the specific pressures of seminary training80 to diverse personality orientations toward faith and doubt.81 The safest conclusion to draw seems to be that we can identify correlations with doubt. We cannot be as certain about cause-effect formulas. Maintaining a sensitivity for these findings, five broad categories were synthesized from religious literature as a response to the query, "Why?" These categories are not meant to be exhaustive but instead to be representative of doubt's possible origins (acknowledging that the "chicken or egg" controversy may be legitimately raised for each of these five categories). 1. Unstable home life. Several researchers have suggested that an unhealthy home environment contributes to potential doubt among its family membership.82 Allport offers one overarching evaluation of home conditions, as it pertains to faith struggles: If, as Freud has said, the religious sentiment is at bottom as an extension of one's attitude toward one's physical father, then we would expect repressed animosity toward this father on occasion to be reflected in a hatred of religion. . . . Probably the truest statement would be that on occasionprobably not often--both belief and doubt may reflect unconsciously one's attitude toward one's parent.83 Helfaer's studies affirm evidence of counterproductive home life in his research on doubt. Specifically, Indies an example of one doubter's immature mother, who regularly expressed strong jealousy toward the father.84 Moreover, Helfaer states that the irresponsible behavior of this doubter's father, in part, precipitated religious questioning in the son.85 To complicate an already unstable parental situation, this doubter confesses that his family was uprooted and moved forty times!86 Consider the broader issue of related emotional and psychological trauma arising from the home life. Albert Ellis and Michael Bernard note that whereas it is a myth that "parents are always to blame" for their children's maladjusted condition, "it appears that parents as role models and reinforcing-punishing agents play a major part in preventing, minimizing, or exacerbating emotional and behavioral problems in their children."87 In this context, Russell M. Grieger and John D. Boyd point out a dozen faulty parenting styles that tend to promote emotional-behavioral problems in their children.88 2. Unhealthy religious character. Certain studies show that one significant element of religious life contrasts individuals possessing extrinsic orientation (that is, people who believe because of social group expectations and/or external reward) with those motivated by intrinsic orientation (that is, people who believe because of personal conviction, regardless of external influence). Using this factor, research indicates that usually individuals who are extrinsically motivated are significantly more prejudiced and rigid in their belief system than the intrinsically motivated.89 In a similar vein, it appears that individuals who are dominated by an external locus of control (that is, they favor the authority and viewpoints of particular people over their own) typically cling to more irrational beliefs than individuals characterized by an internal locus of control.90 Using three standardized testing scales, it was discovered also "that a person who endorses irrational beliefs will tend to be dogmatic and also (end to he religious for reasons of social support and external reinforcement."91 Of these two components (that is, dogmatism and extrinsic religious orientation), dogmatism was found to be "nearly twice as important in predicting" irrationality when compared to the combination of these two components.'" In this light, Chandler researched adolescents who were "especially susceptible to dogmatic conversion"'1' and noted comparisons between their faith struggles and their newfound cognitive capacity to hypothesize (as well as to doubt). Chandler summarizes his findings by saying that one of his more provocative discoveries was that "such dogmatic views are parasitic upon doubt and carry skepticism as their secret sharer."94 In short, dogmatism and extrinsically oriented faith simply set people up for an imminent ambush with doubt. Francis Bacon summarizes this truism when he observes: "If a man will begin with certainties, he shall end in doubts; but if he will be content to begin with doubts; he shall end in certainties." 3. Extreme personality types. Additional research indicates that certain kinds of persons may be more prone to faith questioning than other individuals. A sampling of two of those personality types are discussed below. "Low-Integration" and "High-Integration" types refer to the extreme cognitive capacities to assimilate and/or accommodate life experiences. In studies conducted by Schroder, Driver, and Steufert, a quartet of cognitive levels were discovered,95 including the following:
It appears that for those associated with either of the above two extreme categories, potential for doubt would be greatest. In the first extreme, the categorical "either or" mind-set would be often challenged to reconsider more complex perceptions of reality. Conversely, in the last extreme, the inability to distinguish discrete, cognitive classifications may cause opposite disturbances of doubtexpressed by a call for precision and order and the absence of ambiguity. (A parallel could be drawn between these two extremes and the earlier-noted cognitive extremes contrasting "accommodation only" versus "assimilation only" thinking.) A classic, prime suspect for doubt is the personality type known as the "Perfectionist" (technically described as an "obsessive-compulsive" individual). Of this personality, Freud once observed, "Another mental need, which is also shared by obsessional neurotics ... is the need for uncertainty in their life, or for doubt."'1" Freud proceeded to distinguish the perfectionist as one having a self-fulfilling predilection for failure, because they "turn their thoughts by preference to those subjects upon which all mankind are uncertain and upon which our knowledge and judgments must necessarily remain open to doubt."100 Erikson's studies likewise led him to encounter the potentially dysfunctional state of perfectionism. Tracing the origin of this mental condition to his theory's early second stage ("Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt"), Erikson brands the maladjustment as a "precocious conscience."101 Through further elaboration of such an infant's disposition, he suggests that instead of willfully appropriating things in order to test them by repetitive play, he will become obsessed by his own repetitiveness and will want to have everything "just so," and only in a given sequence and tempo. By such infantile obsessiveness and procrastination, or by becoming a stickler for ritualistic repetitions, the child then learns to gain power over his parents. . . . Such hollow victory, then, is the infantile model for an adult compulsion neurosis.102 Clinical psychologist James R. Beck claims that this pathological personality type often breeds "extensive doubt, fear, and/or relentless rumination."103 Moreover, Beck shows that this condition may reside within nonbelievers and believers alike. On the one hand, Beck shows that the obsessive-compulsive, nonreligious individual may be compared to the person who has a nagging sensation that he may have violated a tax law. His fear of impending arrest plagues him. Concerning more philosophical matters, this type of person may also suffer despair from fears revolving around topics of death and the nature of evil.104 On the other hand, Beck suggests that the Christian who experiences this unsettling condition may, for example, be continually struggling with a Bible passage that "causes him or her to question status with God and spiritual position and place in the future."105 Doubting one's conversion experience or questioning whether or not a particular sin has nullified his salvation are quite typical anxieties for this individual. 4. Psychological factors. Webb posits that all "religious experiences fall neatly into two categories."106 The first (his "Type A") contains experiences that are unexpected and unprovoked by the individual in question. Saul and his Damascus Road conversion would typify this category. The second ("Type B") incorporates all those who actively seek religious enlightenment. The very nature of this latter category, Webb contends, often correlates with certain intentional alterations in biochemical or physical conditions. Drug abuse (even some types of incense), for example, expresses one of the most blatant, causal factors for physiological as well as religious experiential change. Irrespective of his overly simplified dichotomy, Webb still offers believers a sobering word of insight. For his research indicates that less extreme faith practices--even fasting and meditation--are also reported to contribute directly to questioning of faith. Webb convincingly warns that "causing an unusual biochemical state may bring about a change of outlook which can induce doubt."107 5. Life events. The Gallup survey mentioned earlier reveals that there are specific experiences that impact faithtimes when reappraisals of personal belief are often made. The four life events affecting faith determined to be the most frequently cited by respondents include: (1) experiencing the death of a loved one (86 percent), (2) receiving a promotion or honor on the job (08 percent), (8) having a baby (either as a mother or Hit her; 65 percent), and (4) having a "born again" conversion (31 percent).108 It is significant to note that actual experience of such a life event (determined by the respondent's reply of either a simple yes or no) is distinguished from the degrees of influence it had in the individual's life (that is, the respondent's choice between being affected "a great deal, some, or not at all"). Consequently, a second set of complementary statistics should be considered. Eighty percent of those who had a "born again" conversion (again, 31 percent of the total respondents) claim that this experience affected them a "great deal"--the highest percentage in the category of "degree" among this quartet of life events.109 Of that 80-percent figure, 84 percent of the women and 75 percent of the men (yielding a composite of 80 percent) claim that they were greatly affected by their conversion.110 Seventy-two percent of those who had a baby claim "a great deal" of impact regarding their reassessment of life's purpose. Fifty-eight percent of those experiencing the loss of a loved one identify the experience as having the highest level of impact, and only 24 percent say that their vocational promotion or honor affected their faith reappraisal to the same degree.111 "How?" Several avenues of counsel may be pursued when attempting to minister to the doubter struggling with faith. The following half dozen suggestions represent just a few of the possible remedies, not meant to stand as isolated pieces of advice for cure. In fact an intentional integration of ideas has been sought, and, for this reason, a selection of insights from both Christian and secular sources were chosen for their therapeutic benefits. Change Misunderstandings About Faith and Doubt Generally, doubt has been defined as the antithesis of faith, but that is clearly not the truth. As many believers would acknowledge, Towns claims, "I have never met an honest and sincere Christian who has not experienced doubt."112 Yet something can be said for the confusion that arises, because there are so many misconceptions about faith and doubt. In light of this condition, Creel offers three "noetic perversions of faith." (The word noetic originates from the Greek nous, meaning mind; consequently, Creel refers to the cognitive or intellectual misunderstandings of faith. Without altering Creel's intentions, one could easily expand his topic to read "noetic perversions of faith and doubt"). First is the perversion of dogmatism. "Dogmatism, I am com hired, is the attitude of those who cannot live with the ambiguity of human experience--or who don't see the ambiguity (and not seeing it may be a function of not wanting to see it)."113 As a follow-up to Towns' testimony, Creel summarizes his studies: "Most of these Christians wanted to believe with all of their hearts, but unanswered questions plague them."114 Believers often suffer from one particular root of dogmatism: the tension found in theological paradoxes--two truths that seem to be contradictory but actually coexist, much like parallel rails on a train track. A paradoxical example in Scripture would be Philippians 2:12-13: "Continue to work out your salvation with fear and trembling, for it is God who works in you to will and to act according to His good purpose." Here, Paul claims that individual Christians and God Himself are both responsible to pursue the ongoing tasks of salvation. That does not mean that man earns his salvation in any way. But it does point out that regeneration is not an "either/or" matter (that is, God or man) but a "both/and" reality. A person who is overly dogmatic cannot handle this tension and ambiguity. Such a person often suffers from doubt, trying to package Scripture into neat little boxes that never seem to fit. Perhaps it was this understanding of dogmatism that led the skeptic Voltaire to write, "Doubt is not a pleasant condition, but certainly is an absurd one." Next, Creel identifies the intellectual perversion of exclusiveness, explaining to his readers in the second person, "You don't take seriously what the person says who disagrees with you; you only try to figure out how lo convert him. If he proves impervious to your appeals and arguments, then you avoid him."115 In Luke 9:49-50 Jesus rebukes His beloved disciple John, who had earlier restrained someone from casting out a demon. The Lord saw that John's rationale for his restraining actions ("because he is not one of us") was extremely selfish and myopic. Jesus much prefers a far broader category of just two types of people: those "for us" and those "against us." It is fairly easy to see how exclusivistic thinking leads to doubts and false guilt (that is, guilt that comes from man's standards of living, not God's). Third, Creel points out the perversion of coercion: "the willingness to use propaganda or force to make people commit themselves, or at least, say that they are committing themselves, to a certain religious position, whether it be theistic, such as Christianity, or atheistic, such as communism."116 Implications for doubt are self-evident for this perversion, since coercion fosters an unhealthy and unstable state of commitment--to say nothing of the warped ethics involved. Again, the Lord's example is instructive here. In Mark 6:1-6, Jesus returns to His homeland, seeking to minister to family and friends. Upon their rejection of Him, the Lord proclaims the now-famous dictum "A prophet is not without honor but in his own country" (v. 4, KJV). In addition, Mark records that the Son of God was limited to healing just "a few sick people" (v. 5) and that "he was amazed at their lack of faith" (v. 6). The point is foundational: even though this hometown crowd probably included several relatives and friends, Christ refused to manipulate their faith. In fact, His public ministry was quite severely restricted because Jesus affirmed man's freedom to choose or reject Him. His commendable response to Jewish abuse heaped on Him personifies what is recorded of God the Father in Psalm 78:41 concerning the identical, sinful rejection by the Old Testament forefathers: "Again and again they put God to the test and vexed the Holy One of Israel." Emphasize Healthy Thinking Broadening the first suggested remedy for doubt, results in the second recommended task of comprehensive mental fitness. In secular psychological theory, one prominent example of this remedy is called Rational-Emotive Therapy (R.E.T.). The founder of this theory, Albert Ellis, Executive Director of the Institute of R. E. T. in New York City, describes this discipline of cognitive control by articulating its overriding aims:
By way of appropriation, Salzman employs R. E. T. techniques when he suggests that the obsessive-compulsive individual must "acknowledge that anxiety is universal and omnipresent and cannot be permanently eliminated from life. This means abandoning attempts at perfection and superhuman performance."118 Now, it is fair to say (hat the application of R. E. T. in a Christian context is highly controversial--not the least problem of which stems from the fact that the founder confesses to be an avowed atheist.119 However, moderates in the controversy churn (hat the helpful truths present, within R. E. T. can be therapeutic if they are carefully reinterpreted within the Christian context.120 For instance, Lawrence and Huber report, that in one particular case scriptural truth could be intricately linked to R. E. T.'s intentional approach of realistic thinking:
Perhaps one of the clearest demonstrations of the integration between Christian counsel and a modified version of R. E. T. was published in the best seller Telling Yourself the Truth, by William Backus and Marie Chapian (Bethany Fellowship, 1980). CHECK AUTHORITARIANISM Whether in the home or in the church, authoritarian leadership inherently dominates its followers, partially causing the "noetic perversions of faith" that Creel describes. As was shown, coercive techniques were not popular with Jesus, nor are they shown to be productive for healthy human development. Piaget, for example, lashes out at such forms of manipulation because of particular research findings:
Wadsworth updates Piaget's discoveries and offers summary advice for parents and teachers who desire to work with--not against--the God-given nature and pal-terns of a child's growth.
Of course, these insights must not be confined to parent-child or teacher-child relationships. Adult ties (such as those between pastor and parishioner) yield the same payoffs. Consequently, all coercive forms of leadership must be rethought and recast into enabling, collaborative ministries. ENCOURAGE SCRUTINY OF FAITH A twenty-year-old student I know had this to say about using doubt productively: "Use the time of doubts to explore your faith. [Use it] as a time for growing stronger in your faith and knowing what you believe, and why." Contrary to what might be expected, personal beliefs need to be reassessed from time to time. Only a fairy tale view of faith claims that doubts will not surface if--like a hornet's nest--faith remains undisturbed. Based upon earlier statements of faith and doubt, it is not so much whether faith will be scrutinized (for it will) but how faith will be critiqued. Nurturing leaders of the faith, then, must provide a supportive atmosphere where beliefs can be examined. To this end, Allport offers timely insight to the Christian community. Referring to children who suffer from inadequate guidance during periods of faith questioning, he states:
Moreover, McKenzie calls attention to a trio of specific tasks that perpetuate a careful scrutiny of faith. First, religious roots must be explored to enable the individual to appreciate his heritage and provide a framework for his beliefs. Next, religious tradition must be expanded by adapting it to personal life experiences. Without accomplishing this task, heritage becomes irrelevant. Finally--and most important in McKenzie's mind--believers need assistance in the faith-critique process. The author reasons, "If the unexamined life is not worth living, neither is the unexamined faith. ... It is only by means of critical reflection on and evaluation of one's religious commitment that faith becomes truly personal and more than a mere submission to religious convention."125 What is required demands more than mere lecturing about religious catechism. Far beyond prescribed religious instruction, careful scrutiny involves instructional movement over and above formal sets of questions put to growing believers. Rather, honest dialogue with learners meets the challenge of relevant, sometimes impromptu, concerns. Also, unlike most catechetical instruction, the careful scrutiny of faith must involve adults as well as children. Again, McKenzie provides meaningful suggestions concerning such an adult ministry. Along with his contemporary assessment, he offers a word of caution to the church:
Perry Downs of Trinity Evangelical Divinity School echoes the sentiments of McKenzie. Specifically addressing his insights to ministry with adolescents, Downs suggests that the church can serve its doubting youth in three ways. First, a nurturing "context" must be established, or young people might, seek alternate avenues to express their faith questions. "The provision of this context is not as much a matter of program as it is one of at titude"127--an emphatic "I've been there before, myself" attitude. The attitude demonstrates that raising faith questions is not atypical. , Second, concrete answers must be given to at least some of the questions of doubt that are raised. Downs calls for youth staff to have a working knowledge of apologetics (a rational defense of Christianity) geared to the level of adolescent development. Youth need not only "the security of knowing that others have raised these questions" but the assurance "that satisfactory answers are available."128 Finally, Downs stresses the point that those who deal with adolescents
Indeed, the strongest statement that Downs makes to this end of assisting youth through their doubts comes as he concludes that "each of the major theorists who deal with the ministry questions that are related to faith development support these strategies."130 (Among the theorists which Downs had earlier critiqued included James Fowler and John Westerhoff.) Reinforcing several of these factors pertinent to faith scrutiny comes a vibrant personal testimony by one of the daughters of the late Francis Schaeffer. When Susan Schaeffer Macauley was eleven years old, she experienced a run-in with doubt that she never forgot. (Remember Fowler's "eleven-year-old atheism"?) More significantly, she had a subsequent run-in with her father even more unforgettable. Picking weeds one day in the family garden with her two sisters, Debby and Priscilla, Susan started thinking aloud concerning her faith. In their hot, tired, and crabby condition, tempers flared over theological themes. Finally, one of the sisters challenged Susan, saying that her faith questioning illustrated a poor Christian testimony to villagers passing by. "Well, I'm not a Christian anyone!" Susan retorted. "I don't believe any of it!" Needless to say, the dramatic reaction of shocked silence was even more than the preteen intended or imagined. That momentous scene repeated itself inside Susan's young head all afternoon, like a videotape set on automatic rewind and playback. Following the initial shock to even her own system, the questions just would not cease. What did she really believe? Susan was convinced that her public denial would soon be raised again by her sisters. It was just a matter of time and place. Suspense was short-lived.
RESPECT CHOICE AND OWNERSHIP Closely affiliated with the subject of scrutiny is freedom of choice. As individuals investigate what it is that they believe (as opposed to what they are told to believe), caring leadership would do well to value and encourage personal faith ownership. Substantial research indicates that the twin process of self-evaluation and individual affirmation not only possess inherent and immediate virtue but provide necessary innovation for the perseverance of future faith. To this end, respected psychologist David Elkind, elaborating on Piaget's theory, suggests that "it is the child who must, at any given point in time, choose the method of learning and materials that are reinforcing to him."132 Addressing the negative dimension of this educational premise, Elkind continues, "Without the opportunity for student choice and the provision of large blocks of time, in which the child can totally engross himself in an activity, the values of intrinsic motivation will not be realized."133 Wadsworth refers to this matter of choice and selection as "spontaneous interest"--unique reflections of individual preference, often accompanying signs of personal disequilibrium.134 It simply boils down to the fact that everybody prizes participation in areas of personal choice. And faith is no exception. When we "buy into" the ownership of our faith, it is immensely stronger than an inherited--or surrogate--faith. The apostle Paul speaks of the value of faith ownership, using such synonyms as personal conscience and conviction. In Romans 14, rather than winning popularity votes by judging certain cultural controversies as either good or bad, Paul prefers to lay down principles whereby believers can "agree to disagree" with one another. The most prominent principle to surface from this approach is the imperative of personal faith ownership. The apostle challenges individual members of his mature Roman congregation to continually ask themselves, "What is it that I really believe?" and then to live by those heartfelt convictions. Three principles stand out: (1) "Each man should be fully convinced in his own mind" (v. 5); (2) "I am fully convinced that no food is unclean in itself. But if anyone regards something as unclean, then for him it is unclean" (v. 14); and (3) "But the man who has doubts [i.e., who does not have the personal conviction] is condemned if he eats, because his eating is not from faith [or conviction]; and everything that does not come from faith [or conviction] is sin" (v. 23).135 UTILIZE SOURCES OF SUPPORT Analogous to the planned rehearsal of fire drill procedures in a public facility, research shows that most people tend to have a game plan when private faith is questioned. Doubt "escape exits"--so to speak--include ii combination of personal, interpersonal, and supernatural sources. The 1985 Gallup survey uncovers various forms of those game plans as interviewers raised the question, "When you are faced with a problem or crisis, like those in the previous question [i.e., the "life events" noted earlier], to which of the following kinds of support would you likely turn for help?" The question was then supplemented by nine suggestions from the research staff.136 Though limited by the convergent-type format of those nine areas of support, the answers of the respondents were balanced. Most people chose interpersonal forms of support: 87 percent, say they would seek refuge for their faith struggles within their family, and 73 percent state they would share their problem with close friends. A far lower number claim they would go to a religious counselor (40 percent), see another type of professional counselor (31 percent), or discuss the matter with a support group (26 percent) or a religious class (23 percent). One young man recently told me how an assistant dean at his college inadvertently dropped in to see him only minutes before he intentionally planned to commit suicide by drug overdose. This is how he told his story:
Many students have communicated with me how invaluable the interpersonal form of support really is--a nonnegotiable factor for most who mention this theme. On the negative side, one middle-aged woman recalled her doubting experience from eleven years ago. She confided, "No one helped. I quit church for a couple of years." On a more positive note, a younger woman offered this testimony: "During my mother's illness and death, family struggles, and breakup of a personal friendship, the family that I lived with just listened to me, loved me, and continually affirmed that 'God is good find what Ho does is good.'" A twenty-nine-year-old man chimed in: "I saw someone loved me, just as I am--someone showed that he saw some value in me." Just to keep us "church professionals" humble, another young man spoke of his salvation six years ago, with ensuing doubts: "[In looking back] I was amazed to see how little influence counselors, pastors, and leaders seemed to have had in my life regarding major decisions. ... I never met a 'professional Christian.'" A high percentage responded that they would opt for supernatural assistance: 80 percent testified that they would pray about their faith struggle, and 64 percent said that they would rend the Bible or other inspirational literature. (Comparing subgroups, it was found I hat 87 percent of nonwhite respondents as opposed to 61 percent of white respondents made the latter choice.) Also, as much as 80 percent admitted lo valuing a personal form of support, for example, to working out the problem on their own.137 Summary Numerous statements have been offered concerning the nature of faith and doubt. Some of the more salient conclusions of this particular overview have been the following: Questioning faith is a worldwide human phenomenon; it is not restricted to religious or nonreligious persons. Doubt should be viewed from the larger perspective of holistic development; cognitive, emotional, social, and spiritual growth all play a part in the complete picture of maturation. There are different types of doubt, some forms of which may be highly beneficial for faith formation. Because of its genetic (and generic) nature, doubt surfaces within people of all age groups; however, because of its multifaceted nature, it must be addressed as a unique, case-specific activity. Also, the Christian home, church, and school need to produce more knowledgeable and sensitive leaders who will ably assist its membership during difficult times of doubt. In conclusion, Leon McKenzie's earlier quotation deserves reconsideration, for it succinctly capsulates the truth-kernel of Christian doubt and what can be done about it:
1. G. W. Allport. The Individual and His Religion (New York: Macmillan, 1950), p. 113. |